Phonetics and Phonology
Introduction
Introduction
Fichier Détails
Cartes-fiches | 62 |
---|---|
Langue | English |
Catégorie | Anglais |
Niveau | Université |
Crée / Actualisé | 14.02.2014 / 14.02.2017 |
Lien de web |
https://card2brain.ch/box/phonetics_and_phonology1
|
Intégrer |
<iframe src="https://card2brain.ch/box/phonetics_and_phonology1/embed" width="780" height="150" scrolling="no" frameborder="0"></iframe>
|
Créer ou copier des fichiers d'apprentissage
Avec un upgrade tu peux créer ou copier des fichiers d'apprentissage sans limite et utiliser de nombreuses fonctions supplémentaires.
Connecte-toi pour voir toutes les cartes.
The syllable
- number of syllables in a word: easy to determin based on intuition
- syllable boundaries and internal syllable structure: require some analytical tools
Constituents of the syllable
- the only mandatory syllable constituent in English is the core element, the head.
- the head of a syllable typically is a vowel. In English, sonorant consonants /l/, /m/, /n/, and /r/ can also be heads of syllables (bottle, bottom, button, butter).
- The optional constituents of a syllable are the onset and the coda. When they do occur, they each contain one more cosonants.
- syllable structres helps explain why in the phonetically similar pairs [i] and [j] on one hand and [u] and [w] on the other hand, the first element is classified as a vowel and the second one as (an approximant) consonant:
- [i] and [u] can be the nucleus of a syllable, thus they are [+syllabic], [j] and [w] can only occur at the syllable onset, thus they are [-syllabic].
Suprasegmental phonology as a tool for explaining phonological processes
- Without suprasegmental phonology, it would not be possible to predict why /p/, /t/ /k/ are aspirated in repair, return and record, but not in respond, disturb, discard although the position of the three voiceless stops is word-medial in both sets
- Applying supra-segmental-phonology, the respective sounds are considered in terms of their position within the syllable: in both sets, the stops are located in the syllable onset. With a preceding /s/ (in the second set), they behave just as word-initial stops : their aspiration is blocked by the presence of /s/.
Phonetic stress...
- is not an absolute category, but a relative one: syllables are perceived as stressed or unstressed in comparison to other syllables within the same word or string of words
- is the combined effect of a numbe of articulatory features, each one having an effect on perception
the phonetic properties of stress
characteristics of production:
Highter fundamental frequency: vocal folds vibrate more quickly ---> perception: as higher pitch
greater duration than unstressed syllables --> perception: as longe than unstressed syllables
with higher intensity --> perception: as louder than unstressed syllables
- while some words are characterized by a clearcut stressed-unstressed dichotomy,
e.g. /ˈfɑð·ər/, /əˈbɑʊt/, /ˈsɪl·ə·bəl/
(-> stress mark sign (unstressed would be unmarked))
- others have primary and secondary stress:
/ ˈreɪ-ˌdɑr/ (=radar), compare /ˈreɪdər/ (<- schwa and compeltely unstressed)
! note the diacritic signs for primary stress (upper vs lower)
how does low stress effect vowel quality?
It reduces full vowels to schwa
„schwa“= - represented in German as „Stummes –e“
- reduced vowel
Predicting stress placement
- ...easy in fixed stress languages: stress falls on one particular syllable, with the exception of long words (example: gealic word for potato)
example: Swahili: stress on the penultimate syllable. Scots Gaelic: stress on the initial syllable.
- impossible in free stress languages (e.g. Russian) : here, the stress pattern has to be learned for each individual word
- a change in stress can efect a difference in meaning, e.g. Russian ‘muka ‘torment vs. mu‘ka (flour)
Predicting stress placement in English
- challenging in mixed type languages such as English, which has incorporated stress patterns from many languages
- The stress rules (rather: tendencies) in English can be applied to lexical classes:
Verb rule: Stress the final syllable if heavy. If the final
syllable is light stress the penultimate syllable.
examples: /oʊˈbeɪ/ (=obey), /jʊˈsɜrp/ (= to take the lead of sb.),
(-> long vowel)
/əˈtoʊn/ (=atone)
(-> short vowel with a coda)
Light syllables: short vowels no coda
Heavy syllables: all the others
- Noun rule: stress the penultimate (second last) syllable if heavy (long vowel w/ or w/out coda or short vowel with coda). If the penultimate syllable is light (short vowel only), stress the antepenultimate one. (preceding the second to last).
examples: /əˈroʊ·mə/(=aroma), /əˈdʒen·də/ (=agenda), /ˈdɪs·ə·plən/ (=disciplin)
exceptions: /spəˈge · t̬i/ -> long word
noun rule would predict stress on the initial syllable since the penultimate syllable is light
/ˈkæt.ə.mə.ræn/ (=Katamaran) noun rule would predict stress on
-> penultimate
the second syllable since the penutimate one is light
Predicting stress placement: Interface of phonology and morphology
- Suffixation, a morphological process, sometimes alters stress pattern
- Stress –attracting suffixes are -ette (diminuitive) and –ese (denoting a language or a sociolect)
e.g. /ˌkɪtʃ.ɪˈnet/ (=kitchenet), / mʌð·ər ˈiːz/ (=motherize)
-> stressed -> stressed to suffix
Stress-shifting suffixes, e.g. –ic, -ity and –al, move the stress on the base to which they attach one syllable to the right
e.g /əˈtɑm·ɪk/, /ɪˌlekˈtrɪs·ət̬·i/ (=electricity),
-> stressed – „electric“
-> first syllable stressed
/pəˈren·təl/ (=parental)
-> stressed to one syllable to the right (compare with “parent”)
- Compounds tend to be stressed on the first element
e.g. /ˈgrinˌhɑʊs/
Stressed-timed vs. syllable timed
- syllable-timed language: e,g. French
--> timing is based on the syllable per se: an equal amount of time is devoted to each
e.g.: Il é-TAIT une FOIS une PE-tite FILLE qui s‘ap-PEL-ait PE-titCHA-pe-ron ROUGE.
- stress-timed language, e.g. English:
--> timing is allocated on the basis of the distribution of stressed syllables : major stress syllables are required to occur every 0.6. seconds, the time in between is divided up between the unstressed syllable:
e.g. ONCE up-on a TIME there was a LI-ttle girl called LI-tle red RI-ding- Hood. (The last two sequences of unstressed syllables represent the standard in English : two unstressed syllablles following a stressed one. The sequences that have three unstresssed syllables to them are pronounced faster in order to fit into the 0.6. time slot.)
The underlined units are referred to as feet. A foot is the smallest
phonological unit containing an element that receives stress. A
foot minimally consist in one syllable (cf. second line), but can also consist in a
word with a stressed and an unstressed syllable (RAI-der) or a larger phrase
occuring between short pauses that has one stressed syllable and one or more
unstressed ones (the CAT; is IN-ter-es-ted).
Stress and unstress in different word categories
- Content words: words whose main function is semantic (referential to extra-linguistic reality)
(This category also includes compounds , phrasal verbs and verbs with prepositions )
-> have at least one stressed syllable
- Compounds tend to receive major stress on the first element, secondary stress on the
second one.
- Phrasal verbs: have a particle that looks like a preposition, but functions as part of the verb: turn ON, get BY, put OFF. The major stress is on the particle.
- Verbs with prepositions: e.g. believe in, listen to
--> stress is never on the preposition: beLIEVE in, LISten to
- Function words: words whose main function is grammatical
--> are unstressed
Exception: auxiliary verbs at the end of a sentence
or when used in question tags:
You DID‘nt just say what I thought I heard, DID you?
Yes, I DID.
Practical exercise (stress)
- Find two examples for each stress pattern. Write them down.
- When you have done that, check for the content word/function word pattern. In patterns where you have two categories of content words:
- which one receives the major stress?
I promise to leave. -+-+
They don`t react. -+--
Give me a break. +--+
I like you. -+-
Take a bath. +-+
We tried to succeed. -+--+
Check the correct answer
Theirs and ours
are both function words and thus stressed
are both content words and thus unstressed
are both function words and thus unstressed
are both content words and thus stressed
are both content words and thus stressed
Define“ foot“ (stress)
``Foot`` = next phonological unit above the syllable
Please complete:
In stress-timed languages...
....a stressed syllable has to occur at a given intervall
(in english: 6 seconds)
What is phonetics?
The study of the production, physical manifestation and perception of sounds
Branches of phonetics (Kortmann)
articulatory phonetics:
the study of the production of sounds (points and manner of articulation)
acoustic phonetics:
the study of sound transmission (in terms of measurable sound waves)
auditory phontetics:
the study of how sounds are perceived by the hearer
What is phonology?
The study of structural disrtribution of sounds within a given linguistic system:
- which sounds are part of the inventory of a language? (ex: sounds that English has but German doesn't)
- whicih sound sequences are possible within a language? (comp. German and English again)
- what are typical features of stress, rhythm and intonation of a language?
' <--?
indicates syllable that receives the major stress
Phonetics vs. Phonology summarised
Object of study system: sound as such vs. sounds as parts of a sound system
Domain: not language-specific vs. language specific
Descriptive unit: [phone] --> sound as substance vs. /phoneme/ --> sound classified according to meaning-differentiating function
When is a phone a phoneme?
If the replacement results in a change of meaning, the [sound] is classified as a /phoneme/ --> the smallest meaning-differentiating unit within a language.
arbitrary
The relationship between a symbol and a sound or concept it stands for.
Like "the letter a", a phoneme is an abstraction realised in many different forms.
Allophones
Two or more [phones] are allophones of the same /phoneme/ if
1. they are in compemantary distribution
i.e. each one has a clearly defined, predictable environment in which it - but not the other one(s)- occur(s) (word-initially, -medially of finally and / or preceding or succeeding given phonemes)
2. no change in meaning if you substitue one for another
only odd pronounciation
Contrastive phonology and second language acquisition
When acquiring a second language, a new phoneme is easier to learn than a new distribution rule / a new phoneme contrast.
Why?
Easier to fill a previously unoccupied gap in a system that it is to re-arange a system (cognetivly in general terms)
- difficult to hear a difference that does not have a meaning-differentiatting function in one's NL (and therfore ignored on a daily basis)
Give one example for the need of transcription system in cross-linguistic comparisons
Different languages have different phoneme inventories --> clicking sounds, Umlaute in German
Manner of articulation
defined by te relation between the respective movable speech organs, also called active articulator, and the immovable speech organ, called passive articulator.
1. stops (plosives) : complete blockage of airflow followed by sudden release --> /p/ /t/, /k/ /b/,/d/, /g/
2. fricatives: airstream is forced through a narrow gap, causing friction --> /f/, /s/ /ʃ/, /v/, /z/ /ʒ/
3. affricates: subclass of fricatives, start out as stops and "envolve" into fricatives --> /tʃ/ in chin, /dʒ/ in gin
4. approximants: articulators don't approach closely enough to produce audible friction. Quality of the respective sounds is produced as a result of the way the shape of the oral cavity is altered. --> /j/ in yes, /w/ in wet, /r/ in red, /l/ in let
path of airflow
all English phonemes except for /l/ the path of airflow is central
the only English phoneme with lateral airflow is /l/
places of articulation
bilibial - /p/; /b/; /m/; /w/ -> "wet" (using both lips)
labioldental - /v/ -> "vison" /f/ (upper teeth touches lower lips)
dental - /θ/ -> "think' /ð/ -> "that" (tongue touches teeth)
alveolar - / n/; /l/; /d/; /t/; /s/; /z/ (tongue touches alveloar ridge)
post-alveolar - /r/ -> "ring' (tounge is right behind the alveolar ridge)
palato-alveolar - /ʃ/; /tʃ/; /ʒ/; /dʒ/ (tongue is between alveolar ridge and hard palate)
palatal - /j/ ->"yes" (tounge touches hard palate)
velar - /g/; /k/; /ŋ/ -> "ring" (back of tongue touches soft palate)
glottal - /h/ -> glottal fricative; /?/ -> glottal stop
vocal fold action
voiced consonants (v+) --> opening and closing of vocal folds causes vibration
/b/; /d/; /ð/ (-> "the"); /g/; /ŋ/; /z/
voicless consonants (v-) --> vocal folds remain apart, no vibration
/p/; /t/; /θ/ (--> "throne") /k/; /s/; /f/
shared point of articulation
If a group of allophones in complementary distribution all share a bunch of features
e.g. -voiced +oral + fricative: /f θ s/
+ voiced + oral + fricative: /v ð z/
-
- 1 / 62
-